Specialized Neurons Encode Social Learning in Humans

Activity in the anterior cingulate cortex corresponds with observing the behavior of others when their actions, or the subsequent outcomes, don’t match one’s expectation.

By | September 6, 2016

FLICKR, SAGESOLARAt least one type of social learning, or the ability to learn from observing others’ actions, is processed by individual neurons within a region of the human brain called the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), according to a study published today (September 6) in Nature Communications. The work is the first direct analysis in humans of the neuronal activity that encodes information about others’ behavior.

“The idea [is] that there could be an area that’s specialized for processing things about other people,” says Matthew Apps, a neuroscientist at the University of Oxford who was not involved with the study. “How we think about other people might use distinct processes from how we might think about ourselves.”

During the social learning experiments, the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) and CalTech–based research team recorded the activity of individual neurons in the brains of epilepsy patients. The patients were undergoing a weeks-long procedure at the Ronald Reagan UCLA Medical Center in which their brains were implanted with electrodes to locate the origin of their epileptic seizures. Access to this patient population was key to the study. “It’s a very rare dataset,” says Apps. “It really does add a lot to the story.”

With data streaming out of the patients’ brains, the researchers taught the subjects to play a card game on a laptop. Each turn, the patients could select from one of two decks of face-down cards: the cards either gave $10 or $100 in virtual winnings, or subtracted $10 or $100. In one deck, 70 percent of the cards were winning cards, while in the other only 30 percent were. The goal was to rack up the most money.

The subjects learned which deck was better through trial and error—in other words, self-experienced learning. However, they also had the opportunity each round to observe two other “players,” virtual avatars whose moves had been pre-recorded and later adjusted by the research team to ensure an appropriate level of experimental variance. These virtual players chose from the same decks as the subject, and so for every choice the subjects made, they also received two other data points from which to draw conclusions about the decks.

Throughout the card game, as players made predictions about which deck was better and subsequently won or lost their virtual money, the researchers collected data on which neurons were firing and when. “The actual signal that we record is the action potential, which is arguably a ground truth in the brain—it’s really where biology becomes information,” says Michael Hill, a coauthor of the study now based at the Center for Neuroprosthetics at EPFL in Switzerland.

Specifically, Hill and his team were looking at neuronal firing in the amygdala, the rostromedial prefrontal cortex, and the rACC that might encode prediction errors while observing other players—for example, if the subject believed the left deck to be the better choice, but another player selected a losing card from that deck. In the amygdala and rostromedial prefrontal cortex, neurons fired the same way in response to these events regardless of whether the subject or another player was experiencing them. However, neurons in the rACC responded differently while subjects were observing another player versus taking their own turns. “The rACC is highly involved in [this computation], maybe moreso than these other structures,” says Hill, though he notes that the results don’t mean the amygdala and prefrontal cortex play no role in social learning.

The research aligns with previous neuron studies of observational learning in rodents and non-human primates, and with human fMRI studies, says Steve Chang, a neurobiologist at Yale University who was not involved in the project. “This kind of result gives us a good guideline to target similar regions in the animal brain . . . and see what these neurons do when we causally manipulate them,” he adds. “These results from different angles will eventually all converge to an interesting picture for social cognition.”

For humans, the results also point to another potential avenue for research in the clinic. “For conditions such as psychopathy or individuals on the autism spectrum, this sort of hints at a new target,” says Apps. With a team of colleagues, Apps is currently investigating if the social difficulties faced by some individuals on the autism spectrum are due to an absence of activity in response to prediction errors in this brain region.

M.R. Hill et al., “Observational learning computations in neurons of the human anterior cingulate cortex,” Nat Commun, doi: 10.1038/ncomms12722, 2016.

Add a Comment

Avatar of: You

You

Processing...
Processing...

Sign In with your LabX Media Group Passport to leave a comment

Not a member? Register Now!

LabX Media Group Passport Logo

Popular Now

  1. Thousands of Mutations Accumulate in the Human Brain Over a Lifetime
  2. 2017 Top 10 Innovations
    Features 2017 Top 10 Innovations

    From single-cell analysis to whole-genome sequencing, this year’s best new products shine on many levels.

  3. Search for Life on the Red Planet
  4. Two Dozen House Republicans Do an About-Face on Tuition Tax
FreeShip