Menu

Image of the Day: Bespectacled Mantis

Insects wearing 3-D glasses detect computer-generated prey.

Dec 10, 2019
Emily Makowski

ABOVE: Mantis wearing 3-D glasses
NEWCASTLE UNIVERSITY, UK

Researchers fitted African praying mantises (Sphodromantis lineola) with 3-D glasses to study how the insects perceive motion, according to a paper published in PNAS yesterday (December 9).

Animals detect movement in two ways. One is through first-order motion perception, the detection of changes in light over space and time. This can be seen “when a dark bug moves against a bright background,” the researchers write in the paper. The other method, second-order motion perception, is more complicated. Instead of changes in light, it involves more-subtle alterations in contrast across a static background, such as when “wind creates waves of movement over a grassy field,” according to another study.

Because most movement involves changes in light from first-order motion, the authors of the new paper wanted to understand why second-order perception may have evolved. Animal behavior researcher Vivek Nityananda at Newcastle University in the United Kingdom and colleagues looked at how mantises detect prey. The team made small, 3-D glasses with orange and purple lenses for mantises to wear while the insects were shown a pattern of dots on a computer screen. The glasses were necessary for the insects to use stereoscopic, or 3-D, vision to see the computer-generated dots on the screen as lifelike prey objects.

Based on the mantises’ responses to scenes of moving dots and moving backgrounds, the researchers determined that the insects actually depend more on detecting subtle cues from second-order motion than they do on changes in light from first-order. The authors suggest that this may be an adaptation that allows insects to better see camouflaged prey that matches a similarly-shaded background.

V. Nityananda et al., “Second-order cues to figure motion enable object detection during prey capture by praying mantises,” PNAS, doi:10.1073/pnas.1912310116, 2019. 

Emily Makowski is an intern at The Scientist. Email her at emakowski@the-scientist.com.

January/February 2020

A Light in the Dark

Unpacking the Complex Neurobiology of Suicide

Marketplace

Sponsored Product Updates

Distek, Inc. Awarded the Prestigious IDEA Bronze Trophy for the BIOne 1250 Bioprocess Control System
Distek, Inc. Awarded the Prestigious IDEA Bronze Trophy for the BIOne 1250 Bioprocess Control System
Distek, Inc., a leading manufacturer of laboratory testing instruments for the pharmaceutical and biotechnology industry for over 40 years, along with our industrial design partner -  Phase One Design, has been awarded the IDEA (International Design Excellence Awards) Bronze trophy for the BIOne 1250 Bioprocess Control System.
Bio-Rad Enters RNA-Seq Library Prep Market with Launch of the SEQuoia Complete Stranded RNA Library Prep Kit
Bio-Rad Enters RNA-Seq Library Prep Market with Launch of the SEQuoia Complete Stranded RNA Library Prep Kit
Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc. (NYSE: BIO and BIOb) today announced the launch of the SEQuoia Complete Stranded RNA Library Prep Kit, a novel approach to RNA-Seq library preparation.
Inductive Charging Stand from BrandTech® Scientific
Inductive Charging Stand from BrandTech® Scientific
 NEW! An inductive charging stand is now available for the BRAND® HandyStep® touch and HandyStep® touch S repeating pipettes.
Horizon Discovery signs collaboration and license agreement with Mammoth Biosciences
Horizon Discovery signs collaboration and license agreement with Mammoth Biosciences
Aims to develop the next generation of engineered CHO cell lines, to improve production of biotherapeuticsBioproduction is a key business unit within Horizon, this agreement underlines strategy to focus on core markets