The experimental setup that Sponberg and his colleagues used to determine the hawkmoth's secret to low-light visual acuityGEORGIA TECH, ROB FELTHawkmoths are expert hoverers. At dusk, they emerge to float, hummingbird-like, at flowers, into which they insert their long proboscises to sip sweet nectar. But how their tiny brains are able to accomplish this feat—the flowers upon which they dine are often swaying in the breeze—had been somewhat of a mystery. Now, a team has found that at least one species of hawkmoth, Manduca sexta, is slowing down its brain’s visual processing machinery, in a process akin to slowing the shutter speed on a camera, in order to more clearly see the dimly-lit flowers they are targeting. “You’re exposing the visual system to light for a longer period of time before they need to act on the information,” Georgia Tech biophysicist Simon Sponberg, lead author of a June 12 Science paper announcing the results, told The Christian Science Monitor. “You have many frames being taken sequentially and the frames get exposed to light for a longer period of time, but if you expose them to light for too long the frames get blurred together.”
Sponberg and his colleagues showed that the hawkmoths circumvent this blurring problem by only slowing their visual processing down as much as necessary in a given environment. Using robotic, 3-D-printed flowers that moved at different frequencies, the researchers showed that the moths were good at tracking the oscillations of flowers that moved at frequencies below about 2 hertz—which is typical in nature. Above that frequency of motion, the moths had trouble feeding. By pairing their observations with a computer model that predicted the flower-motion frequencies at which hawkmoths would have trouble seeing their meals, Sonberg and his colleagues surmised that the insects were employing the strategy of slowing their brains to feed in the waning evening light. “We found that at exactly those frequencies, the ...